Sucralose: Study Seems to Confirm Pro-Diabetic Effects (-18% Reduced Insulin Sensitivity in 2 Weeks) of Realistic Amounts of Sucralose in Humans - What's the Verdict?

What's the verdict on sucralose?
You will have read it in the SuppVersity news on Facebook (Sunday, September 16, 2018): Scientists from the Unidad de Investigación en Enfermedades Metabólicas, Instituto Nacional de Ciencias Médicas y Nutrición Salvador Zubirán in Mexico City are the first to present somewhat convincing evidence that the insulin sensitivity of healthy individuals can deteriorate (at least transiently) within only 14 days of consuming a high but realistic amount of sucralose (2.25mg/kg total) with every of your three main meals.

Today, sucralose, which has been pulled from the diet drinks of the biggest players in the beverage industry, i.e. Pepsi and Coke (allegedly because people didn't like it - conspiracy theorists listen up ;-), is still used in selected diet drinks, diet sodas, flavored iced teas, fruit-flavored waters, hot chocolate, as well as various sweetened condiments, dairy products, chewing gums, candy and your supplements (e.g. 30-60mg in 50g whey or casein).
You can learn more about sweeteners at the SuppVersity

Aspartame & Your Microbiome - Not a Problem?

Will Artificial Sweeteners Spike Your Insulin?

Diet Soda Beats Water as Dieting Aid in RCT

Experiments Don't Support AS<>Obesity link

Sucralose Tricks 'Ur Energy Gauge - Implications?

Sweeteners In- crease Sweet- ness Threshold
From previous sweetener articles on the SuppVersity, you know that >90% of the studies showing detrimental effects of any artificial sweetener on mammalian metabolic health are rodent studies, which use extremely high amounts of the sweeteners under research (for aspartame, for example, the human equivalent dose is often higher than the amount of the dreaded artificial sweetener you'd consume if you guzzled all 1.5l bottles on a "Diet Coke version" of the Coca Cola's Christmas trucks).

Table 1: Click here to open the very long list of common products and the sweeteners they contain. Among the items that contain sucralose, you will find foods/beverages you wouldn't expect there, like oats or ketchup and several dozens of "sugar-free" products (Spencer 2016).
In the two-week study at hand, that's different: Romo-Romo et al. didn't just replace the 'lab-rats' from the average anti-sweetener study with healthy human beings (N=66, 49f / 17m) with a mean age of 22 years, a normal BMI and a low habitual consumption of NNSs, they also chose the amount of sucralose the subjects in the treatment arm had to consume so that they are actually achievable from dietary sources, alone (see first info-box).

The total daily intake in Romo-Romo's study equaled 15% of the Acceptable Daily Intake (ADI | learn more about the safety of sucralose in Grotz and Munro 2009). For sucralose that's (15 mg/kg/d) or 168mg/d for someone weighing in at 75kg.

Since the authors decided that the subjects should get their daily dose of sucralose from Splenda®, this means that the average subject, whose weight was 58kg (there were more female than male subjects in both groups 24/33 & 25/33, hence the low mean body weight of the average study participant) had to use a total of 11 packets of Splenda® (12mg sucralose, each) to arrive at their target dose of 130.5mg/d.
Based on the limited data we have, it seems obvious that the dosage used in the study at hand (15% of the ADI) is not representative on a population level - for Korea, where corresponding data are available, it is, however, yet representative of the 5% TOP consumers.
11 packets of Splenda®? Is the dosage not totally unrealistic? No, not really. The Splenda® intake of fitness competitors, who obviously don't have to care much about a small-to-moderate reduction in insulin sensitivity, during their contest prep often totals 30 or more packs of Splenda per day.

But let's leave the crazy fitness- and BB-crowds aside and take a look at "normal" people. Unfortunately, there are limited data on the consumption sucralose worldwide. The International Food Information Council estimates the average daily sucralose intake of American sweetener connoisseurs at ~1.3mg/kg (8.67% of the ADI), which is 42% less than the 2.25mg/kg dose Romo-Romo et al. (2018) had the initially 33 subjects in the sucralose group consume.

Studies from other countries support the notion that the dosage the scientists used is not representative of the average sucralose consumption: Buffini et al. (2018) quantify the sucralose intake in Ireland at 2.51% (4.45% if only sweetener consumers are considered), and Ha et al. (2013) estimate the sucralose intake of the average Korean citizen at 8% of the ADI, i.e. ~1/3 and ~1/2 of the dosage used in the study under review. A very recent review by Martyn et al. (2018) who reviewed the published data on the intake of all major low-/no-calorie sweeteners in their article in the March issue of Nutrients adds data for another 7 countries (see table below) - all showing that the average consumption of adults is below the 15% of the ADI line.
Additional estimates of sucralose intake in selected countries in Asia, Europe, and South America | Data based on a review by Martyn et al. represent the average intake ranges across studies in % of the ADI of 15mg/kg, remember the study at hand used 15% of the RDA - world-wide especially children are exposed to much higher dosages.
And no, I didn't forget North America (especially the US). As the researchers point out in their review there were simply "no assessments [...] in North America that presented actual daily exposure estimates to low-/no-calorie sweetener" (Martyn 2018). For many other countries, only total sweetener intakes and/or the individual intake of other sweeteners were available.

Against that background, you will have a hard time arguing that the study results would apply 1:1 on a population level, worldwide. On an individual level, this looks different, though. For high consumers, as well as toddlers and younger children (sucralose has also been found in breast milk), the results of the study at hand are thus very relevant
Before we delve into out analyses of the results, let's first take the obligatory closer look at the actual experimental approach the Mexican researchers used. Even if the dosing seems to be realistic if not for all, then for significant parts of the population worldwide, it is still possible that we can find another important flaw that disqualifies the study results in the first place.

Are there any methodological problems that raise serious doubts about the accuracy or practical relevance of the results of the study under review?

The subjects in both arms of the study, i.e. the active treatment arm in which the subjects consumed 2.25mg/kg sucralose per day in form of Splenda® sachets, the contents of which they emptied into the beverages they consumed with breakfast, lunch, and dinner, and the control arm, followed the same procedures and had to adhere to the same dietary/behavioral principles, namely:
Table 2: The baseline characteristis didn't differ between the initially 2x33 subjects in the sucralose and control group, respectively (Romo-Romo 2018) - most importantly, the two-hour postload glucose load was virtually indentical (the fasting insulin, on the other hand, was n.s. higher in the sucralose group)
"Both groups were instructed to maintain their habitual food intake and physical activity during the intervention period. These variables were evaluated with a 3-d food record for 2 weekdays and 1 weekend day over the 14-d intervention and by repeating the physical activity questionnaire in visit 3. 
In addition, both groups were advised to avoid the consumption of any other products containing NNSs during the study. Adherence was evaluated with a specifc format in which participants registered the number of sucralose sachets consumed each day and the method of consumption" (Romo-Romo 2018, my emphasis).
All subjects reported to the lab thrice: During the first visit the scientists tested the subjects for the most important exclusion criteria, i.e. having diabetes or prediabetes using the subjects' blood glucose concentration as their gauge (obviously, a glucose tolerance test would have been more accurate, but that's no critical methodological issue, because the baseline test would have revealed any subjects whose pre-diabetic status may have been overlooking).
Addendum: Do not fool yourself by assuming that the maltodextrin and dextrose in Splenda were to blame. Both were fully accounted for in the scientists' dietary analysis - with no inter-group differences in simple sugar intake (this has always been mentioned in the article, but a dozen of people must have overread it. That's why it's getting its own red (=important) infobox, now.
During visit 1, Romo-Romo et al. also assessed the subjects' physical activity, and food intake by the means of standardized questionnaires and took their anthropometric measures (weight, height, waist circumference, and hip circumference using measuring tapes), body composition (muscle and fat mass assessed via body impedance analysis (BIA)), and biochemical variables (fasting blood glucose, insulin, and lipid profile).

The subjects' second visit at the laboratory was used to get a baseline reading on the FSIVGTT, i.e. the modified, frequently sampled intravenous-glucose-tolerance test and collect another round of 24-h dietary recalls. Together, the two food questionnaires from visit one and two served to establish the baseline energy and nutrient intake and assure that the subjects habitual non-nutritive sweetener consumption didn't exceed 5 servings per week.

"Bring in your empty and unused sachets, people!" - Is that adequate adherence control?

At their third visit to the lab, the study subjects returned the empty and unused sucralose sachets for the scientists to establish "adequate adherence". The latter was characterized by sufficient compliance (consumption >80% of the prescribed sucralose sachets) and persistence (consumption of sucralose sachets on >12 d of the 14-d intervention period). This may sound somewhat ridiculous, but it's common scientific practice. Moreover, even if we assume that this method isn't sufficiently accurate and (extreme case scenario) only 50% of the subjects actually consumed all 11 Splenda packs per day, this would only mean that even lower doses of sucralose can harm your insulin senitivity.
Figure 1: Changes in glucose metabolism variables in the intention-to-treat analysis (values didn't differ much for the per-protocol analysis); only the calculated insulin sensitivity showed sign inter-group differences (Romo-Romo 2018)
According to the previously stated criteria, the global adherence was 99.1% so that the mean sucralose consumption of the subjects (157.7 ± 12.9 mg/d for men and 123 ± 12.9 mg/d for women) was within the targeted dosage of 15% of the ADI. A dosage of which you can see in my plot of the relative change of the FSIVGTT's outcomes over the two-week study period that it decreased the insulin sensitivity of the subjects in the treatment group significantly. To be precise, the insulin sensitivity (Si) of the subjects in the sucralose group declined by statistically significant −17.7% (−29.3% to −1.0%), but remained intact in the control group, whose −2.8% (−30.7% to 40.6%) decrease in insulin sensitivity was not statistically significant.

What is important to note, here, is that, unlike the insulin sensitivity, the increased acute insulin response, as well as the disposition index and the glucose effectiveness, which worsened in both groups, didn't show the expected significant inter-group differences - a fact that could be a result of the high coefficient variance of the IVGTT, I discuss in the blue infobox below.

Both groups started w/ a mean Si of 5.8 and ended up with 4.9 and 5.3x10-4/min/(mU/L), respectively - albeit with huge intra-group differences.
Is there any chance the reduced insulin sensitivity is irrelevant? To answer this question it makes sense to initially look at the absolute change in insulin sensitivity - a difference of round-about 0.4 x10-4/min/(mU/L) based on the intention-to-treat analysis. This is not that much considering that the intra-group differences, as suggested by the given ranges of (3.4–6.7) and (4.3–9.0) in the sucralose and control group, respectively, and the -30.7% to +40.6% range of the relative changes in insulin sensitivity in the control. Shouldn't this make us question the validity of the test and/or the subjects' overall adherence to diet/activity prescriptions?

Were the subjects indeed adherent or were there differences in their diets & activity?

Well, I would love to tell you, but ('crazy coincidence') the results of the food records, the subjects had to fill, are simply omitted from the full text of the study. The only thing we learn is that the adjustment of insulin sensitivity (Si) by "calories, and sugars consumed throughout the intervention" didn't affect the significance of the results (and yes, the extra dextrose and maltodextrin in Splenda was accounted for). So, if it's not dietary/activity differences the next logical question is:

Is the intravenous glucose test accurate and relevant in this context?

In view of the previously cited extreme intra-group differences, it is only logical to question the validity of the test of the FSIVGTT. According to the "American Diabetes Association" (1998) the FSIVGTT is yet one of two gold-standards (the other being the euglycemic insulin clamp) that 'satisfactorily' assess peripheral insulin resistance - 'satisfactorily' in this context means with a margin of error (CV) of 14-30% (Monzillo 2003). That may sound bad, but since we have to assume that these errors should be identical and random in both groups, they should cancel each other out for high enough participant numbers. Hence, the variability of the test results don't disqualify the results of the study at hand, they could, however, explain why the scientists weren't able to detect statistically significant effects of the other outcome measures.

So the test is great? Not so fast: What we shouldn't forget is a potential lack of real-world relevance compared to e.g. the oral glucose tolerance test (OGTT). After all, the FSIVGTT represents only the response to the appearance of glucose in the blood, not the more complex response that starts at the very moment your sweet taste receptors in the mouth detect the glucose solution you consume in an OGTT,... but I will save discussing that to the next info-box which will deal with the issue "Oral or intravenous - Which will provide more relevant results?".
As I have pointed out in the two previous blue infoboxes, we don't have any real leverage to question the relevance of the data per se - neither because of hilarious overdosing, which has been a problem in many older sweetener studies nor because of grave methodological problems or other major shortcomings. So, if we simply assume that the reported changes are accurate, one approach to answering the ultimate question, i.e "Do I have to avoid products that contain sucralose?", is to compare the 18% decrease in insulin sensitivity Romo-Romo et al. observed in their study with changes that have been observed in other contexts. Here are three contexts come to mind...
  • T2DM patients, for example, see improvements of the insulin sensitivity in the range of ~80% in response to treatment with a GLP-1 analogs (Zander 2002 | this study is also relevant because scientists speculate that the effects may be related to GLP1, the release of which is increased upon co-exposure of sucralose and glucose, Temizkan 2015), while ...
  • sleep restriction acutely reduces the insulin sensitivity of healthy subjects by 13% per hour that's lost (the coefficient is based on reductions from 8 to 5 hours in Wong 2015), and 
  • two-weeks of HIIT training (15 min of exercise; 3x per week; 4–6 × 30-s cycle sprints) improves Si in healthy young men by 23% (Babraj 2009).
In theory, the -18% reduction in insulin sensitivity is thus in a range that physical activity or improved sleep habits could undo, and miles apart from what would turn a healthy human being into a diabetic. The thought that your sucralose-sweetened pre-workout, your post-workout whey protein and the sweetener you pour into your coffee will undo the benefits your sleep routine and/or your HIIT workouts have on your blood sugar control is still annoying, isn't it? Against that background, it is fortunate that not all subjects in the experimental group saw significant deteriorations of their insulin sensitivity. Hence, the obvious follow-up question is:

How likely is it that I am one of those in whom the insulin sensitivity declines, anyway?

If you take a look at the previously reported confidence intervals or the error bars in the figure in the "Is there any chance the reduced insulin sensitivity is irrelevant?"-infobox, you may be asking yourselves: "Would I be the guy with the 30% reduction in insulin sensitivity or the one whose insulin sensitivity wasn't affected, at all?" This is a question that relates to the relative risk of seeing significant reductions in insulin sensitivity in a single individual - the kind of statistical shenanigan you'll see way more often in epidemiological vs experimental studies. In the experimental study at hand, however, Romo-Romo et al. conducted a bunch of statistical analyses that may give us at least an estimate of your or rather the individual risk increase of the average normal-weight non-diabetic, normoglycemic man (21% body fat) or woman (28% body fat) in his or her early twenties who consumes high, but realistic amounts of sucralose over the course of two weeks (this is the point where I should remind you that this is the group of people to which the study results apply - and not to your 3-year-old niece, your diabetic granny or the Olympic gold medalist next door).
Don't underestimate your sucralose exposure. You are probably affected, too! If you think you're not consuming significant amounts of sucralose, take a look at the ingredient lists of your supplements. The label usually won't give you the numbers, a quick research in scientific papers, however, reveals that the amount of sucralose in 50g of commercially available flavored whey or casein proteins is probably in the range of 30-60mg (Pal 2010 & 2011, independent lab analyses); and you can expect similar concentrations in other sucralose sweetened supplements as well as "no-sugar" products you may be consuming on a per serving base (you can find an overview of products and the sweeteners they contain in Table 1).
In order to determine if dietary differences (which were non-significant when the scientists compared them directly) and other confounding factors such as changes in weight, physical activity, calories, and sugars consumed throughout the intervention may explain the deterioration of the subjects' insulin sensitivity, the scientists used a multiple linear regression model. However, even after the inclusion of "other variables that showed a tendency (P ≤ 0.20) in bivariate correlations with the percentage change for Si" (age, BMI, waist circumference, and percentage of body fat), the effect of sucralose was still significant. And...
"[...t]he only variable that signifcantly modifed the Si was sucralose consumption (P = 0.02), with a β-coeffcient of −0.278 (95% CI: −0.419, −0.023) and R2 of 0.254 for this model; the other factors were not signifcant" (Romo-Romo 2018).
Based on the model, the scientists also calculated the previously alluded to relative risk (RR) that a subjects' insulin sensitivity would decrease (comparing sucralose vs. no sucralose, no other parameters considered). The calculated RR for a worsening of the insulin sensitivity of a specific individual in the intervention group was 1.48 (95% CI: 1.00, 2.20; P = 0.04). In layman's terms, this means that the relative risk that his or her insulin sensitivity declined to any significant extent over the two week period was increased by 48%.

Don't forget: A 48% increase in relative risk can be a very small increase in absolute risk

Ha? That sounds huge, I know. Before you flush down all your sucralose sweetened pre-workout, post-workout, whey, and casein supplements down the toilette (don't ever do that, 'cause the sucralose and other sweeteners are also an environmental hazard :-), let's briefly check what this 48% increase may look like if it is expressed as your total relative risk of reductions in your precious insulin sensitivity - here's an example: Let's assume that your baseline risk that your insulin sensitivity will decline over the next to weeks is 2 in 1000, i.e. 0.2%. According to Romo-Romo's study, this 0.2% risk will increase by almost 50%, if you decide to consume 2.25g sucralose per kilogram body weight on a daily basis for two weeks. Now, as bad as that may sound, this does also imply that your absolute risk of a decline in insulin sensitivity increases by 'only' 0.3%. This is, by the way, not a totally unrealistic value, because we can safely assume that the insulin sensitivity of a healthy, lean, active individual like yourself who doesn't change either his/her dietary or activity patterns shouldn't change over the course of two weeks.
The increased insulin levels at t > 60 min in the OGTT suggests ill, the reduced insulin response in the IVGTT suggests beneficial effects of 4 weeks on 200mg of sucralose per day on glucose management (Letrit 2018). In theory, the OGTT has greater practical relevance, because few of us will inject glucose intravenously instead of eating CHO-rich foods, I guess. 
Oral or intravenous - Which will provide more relevant results? As highlighted in the previous infobox, the American Diabetes Association says in an old(er) conference paper that the IVGTT was one of two gold standards for assessing patients' insulin sensitivity.

In view of what recent research tells us about the interaction of nutritively and non-nutritively sweetened foods with the taste receptors in the mouth, the GI tract, and on the pancreas as well as the modulatory role of the microbiome, the assumption that the IVGTT that was used in the study at hand, or similar intravenous tests (a euglycemic clamp, for example) would yield better, or let's say more relevant results than the good old oral glucose tolerance test (OGTT) becomes questionable.

And I am not the only one to question the practical relevance of intravenous glucose tolerance tests, in general, and the IVGTT, in particular. In an editorial comment which has been published at the same time as the study at hand, Pepino et al., who spearheaded the research into the potential negative effects of sucralose on human blood glucose management, point out that...
"[t]he IVGTT is [a] non-physiologic [test] because it omits important regulators of postprandial glycemia, including oral and intestinal sweet taste receptors in the gastrointestinal tract (which could likely be affected by regular consumption of sucralose)" (Pepino 2018).
And guess what, a very recent study by Letrit et al. (2018), who used both an OGTT and IVGTT, confirms just that: You cannot assume to get identical results with an OGTT and IVGTT. And what's more, Letrit's study shows that the tests may even yield contradictory results (see Figure at the top left of this infobox). In their study, the scientists from Thailand wanted to test the effects of 4 weeks on 200 mg sucralose per day (vs. placebo capsules) in healthy volunteers who did not use non-nutritive sweeteners and found: (1) increased GLP1 and reduced insulin sensitivity in the OGTT (same results in obese subjects in Pepino 2013), but (2) a reduced acute insulin response at similar glucose levels when bypassing the taste receptors in mouth and GI tract in the IVGTT - this means that the OGTT suggested a reduced, the IVGTT, on the other hand, an increased insulin sensitivity.

GLP1 could be a mediating factor, here, but the Letrit study didn't assess it in the IV test

Needless to say that it will take more studies to assess, whether the differences Letrit et al. observed between their oral to intravenous test relate to the lack of taste receptor activation Pepino et al. mention in their editorial comment. If they do, however, the analysis of GLP1 levels in an IV-test in response to short-term sucralose administration would be the next necessary step to take in order to be able to explain what exactly was going on in the Letrit study (note: in the said study, the GLP1 levels were only measured in response to the OGTT, not during the FSIVGTT). Ultimately, this may also help us understand and estimate, how "dangerous" the regular consumption of sucralose actually is for healthy individuals.
In addition to the relative risk, the scientists also calculated the odds ratio (OR). In contrast to the relative risk (RR), the OR doesn't compare the risk of an individual, but the odds that there's one subject in the treatment group whose insulin sensitivity deteriorates. It is thus a less meaningful marker for your individual risk than the RR. Hence, you shouldn't be shocked by the reported OR-value of 5.34 (95% CI: 1.39, 20.51; P= 0.01), as it doesn't mean that your individual risk of experiencing a decrease in insulin sensitivity would increase by 434% if you consumed 2.25g/kg sucralose for two weeks, but that the overall likelihood (the odds) that one out of the thirty (after dropouts) people in the treatment group experiences a decrease in insulin sensitivity was 5.34x higher in the sucralose compared to the control group.

Estimate: Only one in 116 subjects will definitely see significant reductions in insulin sensitivity

That sucralose does not pave the way to certain type II diabetes becomes even more obvious when we calculate another statistical value, the Number Needed to Treat (NNT), i.e. the number of people you would have to put on sucralose for one week to see a deterioration of the insulin sensitivity.
Figure 2: If we assume an event rate of 0.2%, i.e. that 2 in 1000 people would see a significant deterioration of their insulin levels within the next two weeks if they didn't use sucralose (this is a more or less random assumption I based on the fact that the avg. human being won't see sign. declines in Si over only 2 weeks if diet and activity levels remain the same), you would have to feed 116 individuals 2.25mg/kg of sucralose for two weeks to find that a single of your 116 subjects suffer from reductions in insulin sensitivity (NNT values calculated based on the OR of 5.34 and a PEER of 0.02). 
To this ends, we will return to our previous example and assume - as we did before - that your individual risk of experiencing a reduction in insulin sensitivity over the next two weeks is 0.2%. If we use this assumption and the odds ratio to calculate the NNT, the number we get is 116. In other words: In our example, only 1 in 116 people (see Figure 2) who consume sucralose for two weeks will 'definitely' see a significant decrease in insulin sensitivity solely due to the consumption of 2.25mg/kg sucralose per day (please, don't forget that these numbers are just meant to illustrate the results, they are based on an estimation of the risk to experience a reduction in insulin sensitivity over two weeks in healthy individuals, and must be regarded as contextualizations that shall help you understand the significance of the results of the scientists' odds analysis, and not as a quantitatively precise result of the study at hand).

The question that remains is: What's the reason for the deterioration of the insulin sensitivity?

If we understood the mechanism, we would probably also be able to estimate the overall significance of the effect in terms of our glucose metabolism much better. Both, Letrit et al. and Pepino et al., seem to believe that the ill effects that were observed in the previously discussed studies (Letrit 2018 and Romo-Romo 2018) were - in one way or another - related to sucralose's modulatory effect on GLP1. Even if that were true, though, we still wouldn't know if this relationship is of a corollary or causal nature; and the contradictory results of Letrit's study rather complicate than aid in finding the underlying mechanisms by which sucralose exerts its medium-to-long-term effects on insulin sensitivity.

In view of the fact that I have another 66 studies related to sucralose, its safety and purported ill health effects to discuss (related to IBS, the microbiome, differential effects according to age and sex, the brain, blood pressure, body fat distribution, thyroid health, etc.), I will address other putative mechanisms in a general follow-up article about sucralose and leave you on the note that a recent (albeit very high dose rodent) study by Bornemann et al. (2018) points to what I currently believe could be an important finding when it comes to forming hypotheses about the health effects of sucralose: Unlike researchers have long believed, sucralose is metabolized in the gut; and what's even more disconcerting is that sucralose itself and its previously unknown (or ignored) metabolites can accumulate in mammalian tissue. These observations lead us back to my humorous "conspiracy theorist" comment from the introduction because a potential bioaccumulation of sucralose was not included in the "decision process for this agent and indicate that it now may be time to revisit the safety and regulatory status of this organochlorine artificial sweetener" (Bornemann 2018). In theory, it is thus (remotely) possible that Coke and Pepsi pulled the plug on sucralose just in time before (I apologize for using slanguage) 'the shit hits the fan'.
There will be a more general follow-up article on sucralose soon. If you don't want to miss it, head over to Facebook, Twitter, or Instagram and subscribe!
So what can we say about the effects of sucralose on your glucose metabolism at this point? Nothing definitive, unfortunately. It is, however, as I've explained in detail in the light blue infoboxes in this article, not possible to simply ignore the results because (a) the dosage was unrealistically high, (b) the test was unreliable or simply irrelevant to the research question, or (c) important aspects of study design and data analysis were generally flawed.

Yes, there's the issue with the non-reported diet and physical activity data; and yes, there's the possibility that the IVGTT may miss important effects of sucralose's interaction with the sweet taste receptors in the mouth and intestinal tract (note: the ones on the pancreas would still be stimulated, so making the argument that there's no sweet taste receptor involvement, at all, in an IVGTT would not be valid).

Eventually, however, none of these 'shortcomings' negate the general validity of the scientist's conclusion that the use of high, but still realistic amounts of sucralose (2.25mg/kg body weight per day, i.e. 15% of the ADI) can negatively affect the insulin sensitivity of healthy individuals within only two weeks. And while this result is at least partially supported by the previously discussed (last infobox) results of Letrit's study, there's, much more research to be done to realistically assess the risk and relevance of these effects, the underlying mechanism, and related questions such as: Do these effects persist, if you stop using sucralose? Does the timing of the sucralose ingestion matter (w/ vs w/out meals)? Is there a linear, logarithmic, exponential, J- or J-shaped dose-response relationship? Or, will other parts of the general population (e.g. the elderly, toddlers and children, athletes, and diabetics) see similar, more pronounced or maybe no effects, at all?

While you're waiting for these studies to be conducted and published, it may, in fact, make sense to monitor and limit your personal sucralose intake. Unfortunately, the contemporary labeling regulations don't require that the food and supplement industry disclose the exact amounts of sucralose (and other sweeteners, aromas, etc.) in a product on the nutrition label. Hence, you will ultimately have to decide whether the (in absolute terms) small risk increase warrants cutting out all products that contain sucralose - a decision of which I hope that the article at hand will help you with. Ah, and I don't have to emphasize that replacing 100mg of sucralose in your diet with the 60g of sugar (sucrose) that produce the same sweet sensation wouldn't be the best strategy to avoid reductions in insulin sensitivity - do I? | Comment
References:
  • American Diabetes Association. "Consensus development conference on insulin resistance: 5–6 November 1997." Diabetes Care 21.2 (1998): 310-314.
  • Bornemann, Volker, et al. "Intestinal Metabolism and Bioaccumulation of Sucralose In Adipose Tissue In The Rat." Journal of Toxicology and Environmental Health, Part A (2018): 1-11.
  • Babraj, John A., et al. "Extremely short duration high intensity interval training substantially improves insulin action in young healthy males." BMC endocrine disorders 9.1 (2009): 3.
  • Buffini, Maria, et al. "Dietary intakes of six intense sweeteners by Irish adults." Food Additives & Contaminants: Part A 35.3 (2018): 425-438.
  • Grotz, V. Lee, and Ian C. Munro. "An overview of the safety of sucralose." Regulatory toxicology and pharmacology 55.1 (2009): 1-5.
  • Ha, Mi-Sun, et al. "Assessment of exposure of Korean consumers to acesulfame K and sucralose using a stepwise approach." International journal of food sciences and nutrition 64.6 (2013): 715-723.
  • Lorenzo, Carlos, et al. "Disposition index, glucose effectiveness, and conversion to type 2 diabetes: the Insulin Resistance Atherosclerosis Study (IRAS)." Diabetes care 33.9 (2010): 2098-2103.
  • Ma, Jing, et al. "Effect of the artificial sweetener, sucralose, on gastric emptying and incretin hormone release in healthy subjects." American Journal of Physiology-Gastrointestinal and Liver Physiology 296.4 (2009): G735-G739.
  • Monzillo, Lais U., and Osama Hamdy. "Evaluation of insulin sensitivity in clinical practice and in research settings." Nutrition reviews 61.12 (2003): 397-412.
  • Spencer, Marisa, et al. "Artificial sweeteners: a systematic review and primer for gastroenterologists." Journal of neurogastroenterology and motility 22.2 (2016): 168.
  • Pal, Sebely, Vanessa Ellis, and Suleen Ho. "Acute effects of whey protein isolate on cardiovascular risk factors in overweight, post-menopausal women." Atherosclerosis 212.1 (2010): 339-344.
  • Pal, Sebely, and Vanessa Ellis. "Acute effects of whey protein isolate on blood pressure, vascular function and inflammatory markers in overweight postmenopausal women." British journal of nutrition 105.10 (2011): 1512-1519.
  • Pepino, M. Yanina, et al. "Sucralose affects glycemic and hormonal responses to an oral glucose load." Diabetes care (2013): DC_122221.
  • Pepino, M Yanina. "The not-so-sweet effects of sucralose on blood sugar control." The American Journal of Clinical Nutrition. 108.3 (2018):431-432
  • Romo-Romo, Alonso et al. "Sucralose decreases insulin sensitivity in healthy subjects: a randomized controlled trial." The American Journal of Clinical Nutrition 108.3 (2018): 485-491.
  • Schiffman, Susan S., and Kristina I. Rother. "Sucralose, a synthetic organochlorine sweetener: overview of biological issues." Journal of Toxicology and Environmental Health, Part B 16.7 (2013): 399-451.
  • Temizkan, S., et al. "Sucralose enhances GLP-1 release and lowers blood glucose in the presence of carbohydrate in healthy subjects but not in patients with type 2 diabetes." European journal of clinical nutrition 69.2 (2015): 162.
  • Wong, Patricia M., et al. "Shorter sleep duration is associated with decreased insulin sensitivity in healthy white men." Sleep 38.2 (2015): 223-231.
  • Zander, Mette, et al. "Effect of 6-week course of glucagon-like peptide 1 on glycaemic control, insulin sensitivity, and β-cell function in type 2 diabetes: a parallel-group study." The Lancet 359.9309 (2002): 824-830.
Disclaimer:The information provided on this website is for informational purposes only. It is by no means intended as professional medical advice. Do not use any of the agents or freely available dietary supplements mentioned on this website without further consultation with your medical practitioner.